themanfromnantucket - There once was a man from Nantucket...
There once was a man from Nantucket...

1782 posts

Introducing Word Lens - YouTube

Introducing Word Lens - YouTube

Introducing Word Lens - YouTube

  • thatsoup
    thatsoup liked this · 1 year ago
  • thisisgettingdifficult
    thisisgettingdifficult reblogged this · 1 year ago
  • somepoetryshit
    somepoetryshit liked this · 1 year ago
  • rastafoogi
    rastafoogi liked this · 1 year ago
  • bbbastiannnn
    bbbastiannnn liked this · 2 years ago
  • getbacktofandomlife
    getbacktofandomlife reblogged this · 2 years ago
  • sleepaspirant
    sleepaspirant reblogged this · 3 years ago
  • dangeroushologramlove
    dangeroushologramlove liked this · 4 years ago
  • traveladventureimagine
    traveladventureimagine reblogged this · 4 years ago
  • kaydenkreates30
    kaydenkreates30 liked this · 5 years ago
  • screamingcockatoo
    screamingcockatoo liked this · 5 years ago
  • smollbeans-bloguwu
    smollbeans-bloguwu liked this · 5 years ago
  • exitnight
    exitnight liked this · 5 years ago
  • soporis
    soporis reblogged this · 5 years ago
  • unfamiliar-void
    unfamiliar-void reblogged this · 5 years ago
  • snail-gijinka
    snail-gijinka reblogged this · 5 years ago
  • snail-gijinka
    snail-gijinka liked this · 5 years ago
  • marinbel
    marinbel liked this · 5 years ago
  • abyssinthe
    abyssinthe liked this · 5 years ago
  • 007hawk
    007hawk liked this · 6 years ago
  • fukindork
    fukindork liked this · 6 years ago
  • the-trustingguard
    the-trustingguard liked this · 6 years ago
  • the-trustingguard
    the-trustingguard reblogged this · 6 years ago
  • kickingkoty
    kickingkoty liked this · 6 years ago
  • carpe-dayum-gurl
    carpe-dayum-gurl liked this · 6 years ago
  • aliencat358
    aliencat358 liked this · 6 years ago
  • motherofallprocrastinators
    motherofallprocrastinators liked this · 6 years ago
  • rjones2818
    rjones2818 liked this · 6 years ago
  • arewetumbling
    arewetumbling reblogged this · 6 years ago
  • user-unknow
    user-unknow liked this · 6 years ago
  • thewatchingangel
    thewatchingangel reblogged this · 6 years ago
  • pumpnypumpkin
    pumpnypumpkin liked this · 6 years ago
  • ladywithawormwoodpen
    ladywithawormwoodpen liked this · 6 years ago
  • 1188ddf
    1188ddf liked this · 6 years ago
  • bargkoj-blog
    bargkoj-blog liked this · 6 years ago
  • theystolemyavatar
    theystolemyavatar reblogged this · 6 years ago
  • theystolemyavatar
    theystolemyavatar liked this · 6 years ago
  • shadywolfflaphands-blog
    shadywolfflaphands-blog liked this · 6 years ago

More Posts from Themanfromnantucket

12 years ago

I don’t under stand racism

because people are like eggs

some have light shells and some have dark shells

but there’s no point differentiating them

because they all taste the same once you crack them open and empty their guts on the frying pan


Tags :
12 years ago
2600 People Form A Chain Celebrating The Anniversary Of DNAs Discovery

2600 people form a chain celebrating the anniversary of DNA’s discovery


Tags :
12 years ago

To say, “This is my uncle,” in Chinese, you have no choice but to encode more information about said uncle. The language requires that you denote the side the uncle is on, whether he’s related by marriage or birth and, if it’s your father’s brother, whether he’s older or younger.

“All of this information is obligatory. Chinese doesn’t let me ignore it,” says Chen. “In fact, if I want to speak correctly, Chinese forces me to constantly think about it.”

This got Chen wondering: Is there a connection between language and how we think and behave? In particular, Chen wanted to know: does our language affect our economic decisions?

Chen designed a study — which he describes in detail in this blog post — to look at how language might affect individual’s ability to save for the future. According to his results, it does — big time.

While “futured languages,” like English, distinguish between the past, present and future, “futureless languages,” like Chinese, use the same phrasing to describe the events of yesterday, today and tomorrow. Using vast inventories of data and meticulous analysis, Chen found that huge economic differences accompany this linguistic discrepancy. Futureless language speakers are 30 percent more likely to report having saved in any given year than futured language speakers. (This amounts to 25 percent more savings by retirement, if income is held constant.) Chen’s explanation: When we speak about the future as more distinct from the present, it feels more distant — and we’re less motivated to save money now in favor of monetary comfort years down the line.

But that’s only the beginning. There’s a wide field of research on the link between language and both psychology and behavior. Here, a few fascinating examples:

Navigation and Pormpuraawans In Pormpuraaw, an Australian Aboriginal community, you wouldn’t refer to an object as on your “left” or “right,” but rather as “northeast” or “southwest,” writes Stanford psychology professor Lera Boroditsky (and an expert in linguistic-cultural connections) in the Wall Street Journal. About a third of the world’s languages discuss space in these kinds of absolute terms rather than the relative ones we use in English, according to Boroditsky. “As a result of this constant linguistic training,” she writes, “speakers of such languages are remarkably good at staying oriented and keeping track of where they are, even in unfamiliar landscapes.” On a research trip to Australia, Boroditsky and her colleague found that Pormpuraawans, who speak Kuuk Thaayorre, not only knew instinctively in which direction they were facing, but also always arranged pictures in a temporal progression from east to west.

Blame and English Speakers In the same article, Boroditsky notes that in English, we’ll often say that someone broke a vase even if it was an accident, but Spanish and Japanese speakers tend to say that the vase broke itself. Boroditsky describes a study by her student Caitlin Fausey in which English speakers were much more likely to remember who accidentally popped balloons, broke eggs, or spilled drinks in a video than Spanish or Japanese speakers. (Guilt alert!) Not only that, but there’s a correlation between a focus on agents in English and our criminal-justice bent toward punishing transgressors rather than restituting victims, Boroditsky argues.

Color among Zuñi and Russian Speakers Our ability to distinguish between colors follows the terms in which we describe them, as Chen notes in the academic paper in which he presents his research (forthcoming in the American Economic Review; PDF here). A 1954 study found that Zuñi speakers, who don’t differentiate between orange and yellow, have trouble telling them apart. Russian speakers, on the other hand, have separate words for light blue (goluboy) and dark blue (siniy). According to a 2007 study, they’re better than English speakers at picking out blues close to the goluboy/siniy threshold.

Gender in Finnish and Hebrew In Hebrew, gender markers are all over the place, whereas Finnish doesn’t mark gender at all, Boroditsky writes in Scientific American (PDF). A study done in the 1980s found that, yup, thought follows suit: kids who spoke Hebrew knew their own genders a year earlier than those who grew up speaking Finnish. (Speakers of English, in which gender referents fall in the middle, were in between on that timeline, too.)


Tags :